Social and Personality Development in Infancy
Chapter 6
Robert S. Feldman
DEVELOPING THE ROOTS OF SOCIABILITY
Do Infants Experience Emotional Highs and Lows?
Basic Familiar Expressions
Remarkably similar across the most diverse cultures
Nonverbal encoding (nonverbal expression of emotion) fairly consistent among
people of all ages
Infant Emotional Expressions
At birth:
Interest
Distress
Disgust
Over next months: clues of other emotions emerge
Mothers report interest & joy by 1 month; most also report anger and surprise
Facial Expression of Emotions
Important nonverbal communication tool used in everyday social interactions
But…what are emotions?
True emotions have three components:
Biological arousal (i.e., increased breathing, heart rate)
Cognitive component (awareness of feeling)
Behavioral components (display emotion)
Infant Emotions & Brain Development
Advances in infants’ emotional life are made possible by increasing
sophistication of brain
Initially, differentiation of emotions occurs as cerebral cortex becomes
operative in first three months of life
By age of 9 or 10 months, structures that make up limbic system (the site of
emotional reactions) begin to grow
Limbic system starts to work in tandem with frontal lobes, allowing for
increased range of emotions
Stranger Anxiety
Stranger anxiety
Memory developsability to recognize familiar people emergesabililty to
anticipate and predict events increasesappearance of unknown person causes fear
Common around 6 months
Significant difference among infants and situations (less anxiety if lots of
experience with strangers, less anxiety with women & children)
A Closer Look…Stranger Anxiety
Separation Anxiety
Distress displayed by infants when a customary care provider departs
Separation Anxiety
Separation Anxiety
Universal across cultures
Begins about 7-8 months; peaks around 14 months
Largely attributable to same reasons as stranger anxiety
Stranger & separation anxiety reflect important social progress: cognitive
advances & growing emotional bonds
Smiling
Earliest smiles: little meaning
6 to 9 weeks:
Begin reliable smiling
Smile first relatively indiscriminate then selective
18 months:
Social smiling more frequent toward humans than nonhuman objects
End of 2nd year:
Use smiling purposefully
Show sensitivity to emotional expressions of others
Decoding Others’ Facial and Vocal Expressions
Imitative abilities early in life may pave way for nonverbal decoding
Infants interpret others’ facial and vocal expressions that carry meaning
In first 6 to 8 weeks – little attention, poor vision
By 5 months – discriminate happy & sad expressions
By 7 months – respond to appropriately matched vocal & facial expressions
Social Referencing
Social referencing: the intentional search for information about others’
feelings to help explain the meaning of uncertain circumstances and events
First occurs around 8-9 months
Intentional search for cues
Aids in understanding others’ behavior in context
How does social referencing operate?
Two views:
Observing someone else’s facial expression brings about emotion expression
represents
Viewing another’s facial expression simply provides information
Do infants really know who they are?
Development of Self-Awareness
Roots of self-awareness
Begin to grow around 12 months
Research
Rouge spot
Average awareness begins 17 to 24 months
Complicated tasks requests
Awareness of inabilities around 23-24 months
Theory of Mind
Theory of mind
Knowledge and beliefs about how the mind works and influences behavior
Child explanations used to explain how others think
How does a theory of mind develop?
See other people as compliant agents
Begin to understand causality and intentionality
Demonstrate rudiments of empathy by 2, demonstrating others have feelings
different from their own; may show care
Begin to pretend and use deception to fool others in 2nd year, demonstrating
others hold beliefs about the world
FORMING RELATIONSHIPS
What is attachment?
Attachment
Positive emotional bond that develops between a child and a particular, special
individual
Most important aspect of infant social development
Provides pleasure and comfort
Understanding Attachment
Earliest animal research suggests attachment based on biologically determined
factors
Lorenz imprinted goslings
Harlow contact-seeking monkeys
Understanding Attachment
The Strange Situation
Ainsworth Strange Situation
Widely used experimental technique to measure attachment
Sequence of staged episodes that illustrate strength of attachment between child
and (typically) mother
Do all infants attach?
Reactions to Strange Situation vary considerably
One-year-olds typically show one of four major patterns
Secure
Avoidant
Ambivalent
Disorganized-disoriented
(See Table 6-1 for summary)
Table 6-1
Does the quality of attachment have significant consequences for later life
relationships?
Yes
Securely attached 1- year-old males show fewer psychological difficulties at
older ages
Securely attached infants are more socially and emotionally competent later and
more positively viewed
Adult romantic relationships are associated with attachment style developed
during infancy
But
Children who do not have a secure attachment style during infancy do not
invariably experience difficulties later in life
Children with a secure attachment at age 1 do not always have good adjustment
later in life
What roles do parents play in producing attachment?
Mothers
Sensitivity to their infants’ needs and desires is hallmark of mothers of
securely attached infants
Aware of moods and feelings
Responsive in face-to-face interactions
Feeds “on demand”
Demonstrates warmth and affection
Responds rapidly and positively to cues
“Interactional Synchrony”
How do mothers learn to respond?
Influences from:
Generational attachment patterns
Infant’s ability to provide effective cues
What roles do parents play in producing attachment?
Fathers
Little consideration of father’s role in early theory
But social norms are changing
Expressions of nurturance, warmth, affection, support, and concern are extremely
important to infant emotional and social well-being
Attachment to Multiple People
Infants’ social bonds extend beyond their parents, especially as they grow older
Most infants form their first primary relationship with one person
One-third have multiple relationships
By18 months, most infants have formed multiple relationships
Parental Involvement
Nature of maternal and paternal attachment differs based on activities
Mothers spend greater proportion of time feeding & directly nurturing
Fathers spend greater proportion of time playing & engage in more physical play
95% of fathers contribute to care, but on average they do less than mothers
Developmental Diversity
Does attachment differ across cultures?
Research findings suggest human attachment is not as culturally universal as
Bowlby predicted
Certain attachment patterns seem more likely among infants of particular
cultures:
Germany: most avoidant
Israel and Japan: fewer secure than U.S.
China and Canada: Chinese children more inhibited than Canadian children in
Strange Situation
Conclusions
Attachment is viewed as susceptible to cultural norms and expectations
Cross-cultural and within-cultural differences reflect nature of research
measure used and expectations of various cultures
Conclusions
Attachment should be viewed as a general tendency, that varies in way it is
expressed according to how actively caregivers in a society seek to instill
independence in their children
Secure attachment, as defined by the Western-oriented Strange Situation, is seen
earliest in cultures that promote independence, but may be delayed in societies
in which independence is less important cultural value
Developing Working Relationships
Relationship development is ongoing process
Communication of emotional states facilitated through mutual regulation model
Young infants are able to read, or decode, and react to facial expressions of
their caregivers through reciprocal socialization
Infant-Caregiver Interaction
Babies & Peers
Babies react positively to presence of peers from early in life and engage in
rudimentary forms of social interaction
Infants’ sociability is expressed in several ways
Earliest months of life – smile, laugh, vocalize
Nine- to twelve-month-olds – present and accept toys & play social games like
peek-a-boo
Mirror Neurons
Mirror neurons
Fire not only when an individual enacts particular behavior, but also when
individual simply observes another organism carrying out same behavior
Help infants understand others’ actions and to develop theory of mind
Dysfunction may be related to some developmental disorders
DIFFERENCES AMONG INFANTS
Characteristics That Make Infants Unique
Personality
Sum total of enduring characteristics differentiating one individual from
another
From birth onward, infants begin to show unique, stable traits and behaviors
that ultimately lead to their development as distinct, special individuals
Erikson: Psychosocial Development
Early experiences responsible for shaping key aspects of personalities
Stage 1: trust versus mistrust
Trust = sense of hope and success
Mistrust = sense of harsh, unfriendly world
Stage 2: autonomy-versus-shame-and-doubt stage (18-36 months)
Autonomy = sense of independence
Shame and doubt = sense of self-doubt and unhappiness
Another View: Temperament and Stabilities in Infant Behavior
What is temperament?
Patterns of arousal and emotionality that are consistent and enduring
characteristics of an individual
How does temperament apply to infants?
Temperament
Refers to how children behave, as opposed to what they do or why they do it
Displays as differences in general disposition from birth, largely due initially
to genetic factors
Tends to be fairly stable well into adolescence
Is not fixed and unchangeable and can be modified by childrearing practices
Dimensions of Temperament
Activity level
Irritability
Categorizing Temperament
Babies can be described according to one of several temperament profiles:
Easy babies—positive disposition, adaptable, moderate or low intensity emotions
(40%)
Difficult babies—more negative moods, slow to adapt to new situations (10%)
Slow-to-warm up babies—inactive, withdraw from new situations (15%)
Inconsistently categorized babies (35%)
Does temperament matter? What matters most: “Goodness of fit.”
Biological Basis of Temperament
Recent approaches to temperament grow out of behavioral genetics framework
Physiological reactivity to novel stimuli
Clear biological basis underlying inhibition to the unfamiliar
Rapid increase in heartbeat, blood pressure, pupil dilation, and high
excitability of brain’s limbic system
Gender-Based Responses by Adults
Parent-child play patterns
Different styles of activity and interaction from parents
Parental gender-based interpretation of child behavior
Gender Differences
Adults view behavior of children through lens of gender
All cultures prescribe gender roles for males and females
These roles differ greatly between cultures
Considerable amount of disagreement over extent and causes of gender differences
Differences between male and female infants are generally minor
Development of Gender Roles
By age 1:
Able to distinguish between males and females
Girls prefer to play with dolls or stuffed animals, while boys seek out blocks
and trucks
By age 2:
Boys behave more independently and less compliantly than girls
May be reinforced by parents’ actions--encourage boys to explore, while girls
are kept close
Hormonal levels may also play a part
Gender Differences--Summary
Differences in behavior between boys and girls begin in infancy and continue
throughout childhood (and beyond).
Gender differences have complex causes, representing some combination of innate,
biologically related factors and environmental factors.
These differences play profound role in social and emotional development of
infants.
Family Life in 21st Century
Number of single-parent families has increased dramatically in last two decades
Average size of families is shrinking
Despite overall decline, half million births to teenage women, the vast majority
of whom are unmarried
Close to 50% of children under age of 3 are cared for by other adults while
their parents work, and more than half of mothers of infants work outside home
One in three US children lives in low income households
Effects of Child Care
Good news: Direct benefits
High-quality child care outside home produces only minor differences
Good news: Indirect benefits
Children in lower income households and those whose mothers are single may
benefit
Bad news:
Infants less secure when in low-quality child care
Children who spend long hours have lower ability to work independently
Children who spend ten or more hours a week in group child care for a year or
more have an increased probability of being disruptive in class